New Social Norms During the Pandemic

Cristina Bicchieri, Sascha Jane Patterson Harvie Professor of Social Thought and Comparative Ethics, found that for people to modify behavior, it’s first necessary to change their expectations about how others that matter to them act and think.

Wednesday, October 21, 2020

By Michele Berger


Cristina Bicchieri, Sascha Jane Patterson Harvie Professor of Social Thought and Comparative Ethics



As COVID-19 spread across the world, it became abundantly clear that different countries were responding differently to the virus. Cristina Bicchieri, who studies social norms and how they evolve, wanted to understand how a nation’s response had affected individual behaviors.

“We decided to do a study in nine different countries, which included Mexico, Colombia, China, South Korea, Italy, Spain, Germany, the U.K., and the U.S.,” says Bicchieri, Sascha Jane Patterson Harvie Professor of Social Thought and Comparative Ethics.

Bicchieri and Enrique Fatas, a distinguished fellow in Penn’s Master of Behavioral and Decision Sciences program, which Bicchieri runs, created a survey focused on how COVID-related norms like social distancing and mask-wearing have emerged. Broadly, they found that changes come about under three conditions, two of which relate to expectations about the actions and beliefs of relevant others. These “others” aren’t just family, friends, and neighbors, but also people who live in the same city or county, and generally people whose behavior matters to us.

“Public information and the media may change people’s expectations about what others do and what others believe is appropriate to do,” she says. But new expectations alone aren’t enough; they need to cause people to want to change their behavior.

To test this in the context of COVID, Bicchieri and her team presented vignettes to subjects from all nine countries, varying the expectations of the vignette protagonist, then asked participants the likelihood that person would practice social distancing and stay home.

The researchers found that to motivate people to modify their behavior, it’s first necessary to change their expectations. “This is very important,” Bicchieri says. “It’s not enough to say that sending a message about what others do or approve of will induce a behavioral change. We want to be sure that these social expectations actually push people to behave in a different way.”

But is changing expectations enough? In a public health crisis like the COVID-19 pandemic, Bicchieri adds it’s crucial not to underestimate the importance of trust in science. When people don’t trust the science, they may be tempted to free-ride on the behavior of others because they believe their own risk to be low, she says.

We live in a world so globalized that pandemics will happen more than once. We have to be prepared to try to change people’s behaviors. There is a lot of work to be done.

She offers several recommendations the research has shown can make norm-nudging successful: In circumstances like the pandemic, governments shouldn’t downplay science, nor should they send conflicting messages. Beyond that, they should shape their message for the audience they want to reach.

Bicchieri gives the example of young people minimizing their risk of getting COVID-19. “A common message in both Italy and the U.S. was, ‘Older people and people with pre-existing conditions are the most vulnerable.’ A lot of young people thought, ‘I’m not old, I don’t have a pre-existing disease, therefore it’s safe for me,’” she says. “You have to consider tailoring different messages and changing how you send these messages to different groups.”

That speaks to Bicchieri’s final point: In communication, showing what people actually do far outperforms telling what they approve of. Describe a person who has accepted the lockdown and follows social distancing rules, for example, and most people will infer such a person approves of these behaviors. That same inference doesn’t happen when someone is simply described as supporting the measures. “Words and deeds are different,” Bicchieri says. “We may approve of something and yet still be tempted not to do it. But if we do something, we tend to approve of it.”

Future work in this realm will consider whether gender, income, or level of instruction matter in eliciting behavior change. “We live in a world so globalized that pandemics will happen more than once,” she says. “We have to be prepared to try to change people’s behaviors. There is a lot of work to be done.”